Understanding Trauma Compulsions – Psychology of Scams

When exploring the psychology of scams it is essential to look deeply at trauma and its compulsions and how they manifest

The Compulsion to Repeat the Trauma

“Re-enactment, Revictimization, and Masochism”

By Bessel A. van der Kolk, MD*

Published in: Psychiatric Clinics of North America, Volume 12, Number 2, Pages 389-411, June 1989.

SCARS Editor’s Note:

This research is not specifically about scams but is about the trauma that scam victims can experience. Trauma, which can be caused by an infinite number of sources manifests in extraordinarily similar ways. But it also results in compulsions that victims will need to understand. Thus the insights provided in this document can help inform victims’ assistance care providers in their support of relationship scam victims. It can also provide an understanding for victims, and provide them with topics to explore with their mental healthcare providers during counseling or therapy.

We acknowledge that this is not the only source of insights on the subject, but does provide a rich set of materials to learn from and apply in supporting scam victims.

This document has been edited for spelling and grammar and formatted for this medium.

IMPORTANT PSYCHOLOGICAL WARNING

This document is presented for educational purposes only and is not intended to diagnose, treat, or cure any disease or mental illness. If you or someone you know feels that they have been traumatized we urge you or them to seek appropriate licensed medical or mental healthcare immediately. All such care should be provided by licensed professionals only. Victims’ assistance and support groups may assist in the support of victims of crime as long as they are suitable registered professions and considered in the context of supportive care and should not be considered a replacement for mental healthcare. Never listen to or accept the advice of amateurs. To locate a trauma professional in your area click here: https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/therapists/trauma-and-ptsd

INTRODUCTION

During the formative years of contemporary psychiatry, much attention was paid to the continuing role of past traumatic experiences on the current lives of people. Charcot, Janet, and Freud all noted that fragmented memories of traumatic events dominated the mental life of many of their patient and built their theories about the nature and treatment of psychopathology on this recognition. Janet75 thought that traumatic memories of traumatic events persist as unassimilated fixed ideas that act as foci for the development of alternate states of consciousness, including dissociative phenomena, such as fugue states, amnesias, and chronic states of helplessness and depression. Unbidden memories of the trauma may return as physical sensations, horrific images or nightmares, behavioral reenactments, or a combination of these. Janet showed how traumatized individuals become fixated on the trauma: difficulties in assimilating subsequent experiences as well. It is “as if their personality development has stopped at a certain point and cannot expand anymore by the addition or assimilation of new elements.”76 Freud independently came to similar conclusions.43,45 Initially, he thought all hysterical symptoms were caused by childhood sexual “seduction” of which unconscious memories were activated when during adolescence, a person was exposed to situations reminiscent of the original trauma. The trauma permanently disturbed the capacity to deal with other challenges, and the victim who did not integrate the trauma was doomed to “repeat the repressed material as a contemporary experience in instead or . . . remembering it as something belonging to the past.”44 In this article, I will show how the trauma is repeated on behavioral, emotional, physiologic, and neuroendocrinological levels, whose confluence explains the diversity of repetition phenomena.

Many traumatized people expose themselves, seemingly compulsively, to situations reminiscent of the original trauma. These behavioral reenactments are rarely consciously understood to be related to earlier life experiences. This “repetition compulsion” has received surprisingly little systematic exploration during the 70 years since its discovery, though it is regularly described in the clinical literature.12,17,21,29,61,64,65,69,88,112,137 Freud thought that the aim of repetition was to gain mastery, but clinical experience has shown that this rarely happens; instead, repetition causes further suffering for the victims or for people in their surroundings.

Children seem more vulnerable than adults to compulsive behavioral repetition and loss of conscious memory of the trauma.70,136. However, responses to projective tests show that adults, too, are liable to experience a large range of stimuli vaguely reminiscent of the trauma as a return of the trauma itself and to react accordingly.39,42

BEHAVIORAL RE-ENACTMENT

In a behavioral re-enactment of the trauma, the self may play the role of either victim or victimizer.

Harm to Others

Re-enactment of victimization is a major cause of violence. Criminals have often been physically or sexually abused as children.55,121 In a recent prospective study of 34 sexually abused boys, Burgess et al.20 found a link with drug abuse, juvenile delinquency, and criminal behavior only a few year later. Lewis89,91 has extensively studied the association between childhood abuse and subsequent victimization of others. Recently, she showed that of 14 juveniles condemned to death for murder in the United States in 1987, 12 had been brutally physically abused, and five had been sodomized by relatives.90 In a study of self-mutilating male criminals, Brach-y-Rita7 concluded that “the constellation of withdrawal, depressive reaction, hyperreactivity, stimulus-seeking behavior, impaired pain perception, and violent aggressive behavior directed at self or others may be the consequence of having been reared under conditions of maternal social deprivation. This constellation of symptoms is a common phenomenon among a member of environmentally deprived animals.”

Self-destructiveness

Self-destructive acts are common in abused children. Green53,54 found that 41 percent of his sample of abused children engaged in headbanging, biting, burning, and cutting. In a controlled, double-blind study on traumatic antecedents of borderline personality disorder, we found a highly significant relationship between childhood sexual abuse and various kinds of self-harm later in life, particularly cutting and self-starving.143a Clinical reports also consistently show that self-mutilators have childhood histories of physical or sexual abuse or repeated surgery.52,106,118,126 Simpson and Porter126 found a significant association between self-mutilation and other forms of self-deprecation or self-destruction such as alcohol and drug abuse and eating disorders. They sum up the conclusions of many students of this problem in stating that “self-destructive activities were not primarily related to conflict, guilt and superego pressure, but to more primitive behavior patterns originating in painful encounters with hostile caretakers during the first years of life.”

Revictimization

Revictimization is a consistent finding.35,47,61 Victims of rape are more likely to be raped and women who were physically or sexually abused as children are more likely to be abused as adults. Victims of child sexual abuse are at high risk of becoming prostitutes.38,72,125 Russell,120 in a very careful study of the effects of incest on the life of women, found that few women made a conscious connection between their childhood victimization and their drug abuse, prostitution, and suicide attempts. Whereas 38 percent of a random sample of women reported incidents of rape or attempted rape after age 14, 68 percent of those with a childhood history of incest did. Twice as many women with a history of physical violence in their marriages (27 percent), and more than twice as many (53 percent) reported unwanted sexual advances by an unrelated authority figure such as a teacher, clergyman, or therapist. Victims of father-daughter incest were four times more likely than non-incest victims to be asked to pose for pornography.

RE-EXPERIENCING AFTER ADULT TRAUMA

There are sporadic clinical reports,12,59 but systematic studies on re-enactment and revictimization in traumatized adults are even scarcer than in children. In one study of adults who had recently been in accidents,68 57 percent showed behavioral re-enactments, and 51 percent had recurrent intrusive images. In this study, the frequency with which recurrent memories were experienced on a somatic level, as panic and anxiety attacks, was not examined. Studies of burned children131 and adult survivors of natural and manmade disasters67,124 show that, over time, recurrent symbolic or visual recollections and behavioral re-enactments abate, but there is often persistent chronic anxiety that can be interpreted as partial somatosensory reliving, dissociated from visual or linguistic representations of the trauma.141 There are scattered clinical reports64,65,109 of people re-enacting the trauma on its anniversary. For example, we treated a Vietnam veteran who had lit a cigarette at night and caused the death of a friend by a Vietcong sniper’s bullet in 1968. From 1969 to 1986, on the exact anniversary of the death, to the hour and minute, he yearly committed “armed robbery” by putting a finger in his pocket and staging a “holdup,” in order to provoke gunfire from the police. The compulsive re-enactment ceased when he came to understand its meaning.

SOCIAL ATTACHMENT AND THE TRAUMA RESPONSE

Human beings are strongly dependent on social support for a sense of safety, meaning, power, and control.14,15,93 Even our biologic maturation is strongly influenced by the nature of early attachment bonds.137 Traumatization occurs when both internal and external resources are inadequate to cope with external threats. Physical and emotional maturation, as well as innate variations in physiologic reactivity to perceived danger, play important roles in the capacity to deal with external threats.77 The presence of familiar caregivers also plays an important role in helping children modulate their physiologic arousal.146 In the absence of a caregiver, children experience extremes of under-and over-arousal that are physiologically aversive and disorganizing.38 The availability of a caregiver who can be blindly trusted when their own resources are inadequate is very important in coping with threats. If the caregiver is rejecting and abusive, children are likely to become hyperaroused. When the persons who are supposed to be the sources of safety and nurturance become simultaneously the sources of danger against which protection is needed, children maneuver to re-establish some sense of safety. Instead of turning on their caregivers and thereby losing hope for protection, they blame themselves. They become fearfully and hungrily attached and anxiously obedient.24 Bowlby16 calls this “a pattern of behavior in which avoidance of them competes with his desire for proximity and care and in which angry behavior is apt to become prominent.”

Studies by Bowlby and Ainsworth1 in humans, and by Harlow and his heirs58,114 in other primates, demonstrate the crucial role that a “safe base” plays for normal social and biologic development. As children mature, they continually acquire new cognitive schemata in which to frame current life experiences. These ever-expanding cognitive schemes decrease their reliance on the environment for soothing and increase their own capacity to modulate physiologic arousal in the face of threat. Thus, the cognitive preparedness (development) of an individual interacts with the degree of physiologic disorganization to determine the capacity for mental processing of potentially traumatizing experiences.137,141

SEX DIFFERENCES

The frequency with which abused children repeat aggressive interactions has suggested to Green53 a link between the compulsion to repeat and identification with the aggressor, which replaces fear and helplessness with a sense of omnipotence. There are significant sex differences in the way trauma victims incorporate the abuse experience. Studies by Carmen et al.22,71 and others indicate that abused men and boys tend to identify with the aggressor and later victimize others whereas abused women are prone to become attached to abusive men who allow themselves and their offspring to be victimized further.

Reiker and colleagues113 have pointed out that “confrontations with violence challenges one’s most basic assumptions about the self as invulnerable and intrinsically worthy and about the world as orderly and just. After abuse, the victim’s view of self and world can never be the same again: it must be reconstructed.to incorporate the abuse experience.” Assuming responsibility for the abuse allows feelings of helplessness to be replaced with an illusion of control. Ironically, victims of rape who blame themselves have a better prognosis than those who do not assume this false responsibility: it allows the locus of control to remain internal and prevent helplessness. Children are even more likely to blame themselves: “The child needs to hold on to an image of the parent as good in order to deal with the intensity of fear and rage which is the effect of the tormenting experiences.”113 Anger directed against the self or others is always a central problem in the life of people who have been violated. Reikers concludes that “this ‘acting out’ is seldom understood by either victims or clinicians as being a repetitive re-enactment of real events from the past.”

THE SEPARATION REPONSE

Primates have evolved highly complex ways to maintain attachment bonds; they are intensely dependent on their caregivers at the start. In lower primates, his dependency is principally expressed in physical contact, in humans, this is supplemented by verbal communication. McLean93 suggests that language is an evolutionary development from the mammalian separation cry that induces caregivers to provide safety, nurturance, and social stimulation. Primates react to separation from attachment figures as if they were directly threatened. Thus, small children, unable to anticipate the future, experience separation anxiety as soon as they lose sight of their mothers. Bowlby has described the protest and despair phases of this response in great detail.14,15 As people mature, they develop an ever-enlarging repertoire of coping responses, but adults are still intensely dependent upon social support to prevent and overcome traumatization, and under threat, they still may cry out for their mothers.57 Sudden, uncontrollable loss of attachment bonds is an essential element in the development of post-traumatic stress syndromes.45,88,92,138 On exposure to extreme terror, even mature people have protested and despair responses (anger and grief, intrusion and numbing) that make them turn toward the nearest available source of comfort to return to a state of both psychological and physiologic calm. Thus, the severe external threat may result in renewed clinging and neophobia in both children and adults.8,41,111 Because the attachment system is so important, mobilization of social supports is an important element in the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).

INCREASED ATTACHMENT IN THE FACE OF DANGER

People in general, and children in particular, seek increased attachment in the face of external danger. Pain, fear, fatigue, and loss of loved ones and protectors all evoke efforts to attract increased care,8,41,111 and most cultures have rituals designed to provide it. When there is no access to ordinary sources of comfort, people may turn toward their tormentors.14,38,80,102 Adults, as well as children, may develop strong emotional ties with people who intermittently harass, beat, and threaten them. Hostages have put up bail for their captors, expressed a wish to marry them, or had sexual relations with them;31 abused children often cling to their parents and resist being removed from the home;31,80 inmates of Nazi prison camps sometimes imitated their captors by sewing together clothing to copy SS uniforms.11 When Harlow observed this in nonhuman primates, he stated that “the immediate consequences of maternal rejection is the accentuation of proximity seeking on the part of the infant.”114

Walker145 and Dutton and Painter31 have noted that the bond between batter and victim in abusive marriages resembles the bond between captor and hostage or cult leader and follower. Social workers, police, and legal personnel are constantly frustrated by the strength of this bond. The woman’s longing for the batterer soon prevails over memories of the terror, and she starts to make excuses for his behavior. This pattern is so common that women engaged in these sorts of relationships become the recipients of intense anger for social service personnel. They are then called masochistic, and like other psychiatric terms, this can be employed pejoratively rather than conveying an understanding of the underlying causes and treatment of the problem. Walker145 first applied ethnology to the study of traumatic bonding in such couples. A central component is captivity, the lack of permeability, and the absence of outside support or influence.31,62,119,145 The victim organizes her life completely around pleasing her captor and his demands. As Dutton and Painter point out, “her compliance legitimates his demands, builds up a store of repressed anger and frustration on her part (which may surface in her goading him or fighting back during an actual argument, leading to escalating violence), and systematically eliminates opportunities for her to build up a supportive network which could eventually assist her in leaving the relationship.”

Walker145 has clarified the operation of intermittent reinforcement paradigms in such relationships, applying the animal model of punishment-indulgence patterns. In child abuse or spouse battering, this mechanism is accentuated by the extreme contrast of terror followed by submission and reconciliation. When such negative reinforcement occurs intermittently, the reinforced response consolidates the attachment between victim and victimizer. During the abuse, victims tend to dissociate emotionally with a sense of disbelief that the incident is really happening. This is followed by the typical post-traumatic response of numbing and constriction, resulting in inactivity, depression, self-blame, and feelings of helplessness. Walker145 describes the process as follows: “tension gradually builds” (during phase one), an explosive battering incident occurs (during phase two), and a “calm, loving respite follows phase three). The violence allows intense emotional engagement and dramatic scenes of forgiveness, reconciliation, and physical contact that restores the fantasy of fusion and symbiosis.87,140 Hence, there are two powerful sources of reinforcement: the “arousal-jag” or excitement before the violence and the peace of surrender afterward, Both of these responses, placed at appropriate intervals, reinforce the traumatic bond between victim and abuser.31,145 To varying degrees, the memory of the battering incidents is state-dependent or dissociated, and thus only comes back in full force during renewed situations of terror. This interferes with good judgment about the relationship and allows longing for love and reconciliation to overcome realistic fears.

VULNERABILITY TO DEVELOP TRAUMATIC BONDING

At least four studies of family violence40,48,63,132 have found a direct relationship between the severity of childhood physical abuse and later marital violence. Interestingly, nonhuman primates subjected to early abuse and deprivation also are more likely to engage in violent relationships with their peers as adults.134 as in humans, males tend to be hyperaggressive, and females fail to protect themselves and their offspring against danger. Neither sex develops the capacity for sustained peaceful social interactions.134

People who are exposed early to violence or neglect come to expect it as a way of life. They see the chronic helplessness of their mothers and fathers’ alternating outbursts of affection and violence; they learn that they themselves have no control. As adults, they hope to undo the past by love, competency, and exemplary behavior.46,87,145 When they fail they are likely to make sense out of this situation by blaming themselves. When they have little experience with nonviolent resolution of differences, partners in relationships alternate between an expectation of perfect behavior leading to perfect harmony and a state of helplessness, in which all verbal communication seems futile. A return to earlier coping mechanisms, such as self-blame, numbing (by means of emotional withdrawal or drugs or alcohol), and physical violence sets the stage for a repetition of the childhood trauma and “return of the repressed.”1,42,46,137

BIOLOGIC RESPONSES TO TRAUMATIZATION

Chronic physiologic hyperarousal to stimuli reminiscent of the trauma is a cardinal feature of the trauma response, well documented in a large variety of traumatized individuals, including victims of child abuse, burns, rape, natural disasters, and war.2,78,84,107,133,142 Because of their decreased capacity to modulate physiologic arousal, which leads to reduced ability to utilize symbols and fantasy to cope with stress, they tend to experience later stresses as somatic states, rather than as specific events that require specific means of coping.142 Thus, victims of trauma respond to contemporary stimuli as if the trauma had returned, without conscious awareness that past injury rather than current stress is the basis of their physiologic emergency responses. The hyperarousal interferes with their ability to make calm and rational assessments and prevents resolution and integration of the trauma.142 They respond to threats as emergencies requiring action rather than thought.

Chronic hyperarousal in response to new challenges is also found in animals exposed to inescapable shock.5 In fact, this phenomenon drew our attention to the possibility of using this animal model for the study of human traumatization.142 Human beings and other mammals are very similar biologically in respect to such relatively uncomplicated behaviors as fight, flight, and freeze responses. Exposure to inescapable aversive events has widespread behavioral and physiologic effects on animals including (1) deficits in learning to escape novel adverse situations, (2) decreased motivation for learning new options, (3) chronic subjective distress,94 and (4) increased tumor genesis and immunosuppression.143 All this is the result not of the shock itself but of a helplessness syndrome that is a result of the lack of control that the animal has in terminating shock.

Several neurotransmitters have been shown to be affected by inescapably fearful experiences in animals; they have low resting cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) norepinephrine, but under stress, they respond with much higher elevations than other animals. Something has disturbed the organism’s capacity to modulate the extent of arousal.37,95,115,116,142 Dysregulation of the serotonin system has been implicated in this.123,139 Serotonin is thought to be the neurotransmitter most involved in modulating the actions of other neurotransmitters;19 it has also been implicated in the fine-tuning of emotional reactions, particularly arousal and aggression.18 Traumatization also causes dysregulation of the endogenous opioid system in both animals and humans. We will discuss this phenomenon and how this could explain the clinical phenomenon of compulsive re-exposure to trauma.

STATE-DEPENDENT LEARNING

Both Janet74 and Freud observed that early memory traces can be activated by later events that cause partial reliving of earlier traumas in the form of affect states, anxiety, or re-enactments. Their patients generally had a poor memory for traumatic childhood events, until they were brought back, by means of hypnosis, to a state of mind similar to the one they were in at the time of the trauma. In the past few decades, these notions have gained scientific confirmation with the discovery of state-dependent learning; for example, what is learned under the influence of a particular drug tends to become dissociated and seemingly lost until the return of the state similar to the one in which the memory was stored. State dependency can be roughly related to arousal levels. For example, state-dependent learning in humans is produced by both psychostimulants and depressants: alcohol, marijuana, barbiturates, and amphetamines, as well as other psychoactive agents.32 Reactivation of past learning, is relatively automatic: contextual stimuli directly evoke memories without conscious awareness of the transition. The more similar are the contextual stimuli are to conditions prevailing at the time of the original storage of memories, the more likely the probability of retrieval. Both internal states, such as particular effects, or external events reminiscent of earlier trauma thus can trigger a return to feeling as if victims are back in their original traumatizing situation. Thus, battered women who otherwise behave competently may experience themselves within the battering relationship like the terrified child they once were in a violent or alcoholic home.119 Similarly, war veterans may be asymptomatic until they become intimate with a partner and start reliving feelings of loss, grief, vulnerability, and revenge related to the death of a comrade on the battlefield but that are now incorrectly attributed to some element of the current relationship. Disinhibition resulting from drugs or alcohol strongly facilitates the occurrence of such reliving experiences, which then may take the form of acting out violent or sexual traumatic episodes.107

During states of massive autonomic arousal, memories are laid down that powerfully influence later actions and interpretations of events. Long-term activation of memory tracts is observed in animals exposed to a highly stressful stimulus.51,81 This phenomenon has been attributed to massive noradrenergic activity at the time of the stress.129 In traumatized people, visual and motoric reliving experiences, nightmares, flashbacks, and re-enactments are generally preceded by physiologic arousal.30 Activation of long-term augmented memory tracts may explain why current stress is experienced as a return of the trauma.

“RETURN OF THE REPRESSED” OCCURS IN SITUATIONS OF THREAT

Under ordinary conditions, most previously traumatized individuals can adjust psychologically and socially. Studies have shown this to be true of victims of rape,82 battered women,63 and victims of child abuse.53 Nonhuman primates subjected to extended periods of isolation may later become reasonably well integrated socially. However, they do not respond to stress in the same ways as their non-traumatized peers. Studies in the Wisconsin primate laboratory have shown that, even after an initial good social adjustment, heightened emotional or physical arousal causes social withdrawal or aggression.86 Even monkeys that recover in other respects tend to respond inappropriately to sexual arousal and misperceive social cues when threatened by a dominant animal.4,95,101 Animals with a history of trauma also have much more intense catecholamine responses to stress85 and a blunted cortisol response.25

Stress causes a return to earlier behavior patterns throughout the animal kingdom. In experiments in mice, Mitchell and colleagues98,99 found that arousal state determines how an animal will react to stimuli. In a state of low arousal, animals tend to be curious and seek novelty. During high arousal, they are frightened, avoid novelty, and perseverate in familiar behavior regardless of the outcome. Under ordinary circumstances, an animal will choose the most pleasant of two alternatives. When hyperaroused, it will seek the familiar, regardless of the intrinsic rewards.99 Thus shocked animals returned to the box in which they were originally shocked, in preference to less familiar locations not associated with punishment. Punished animals actually increased their exposure to shock as the trials continued.98 Mitchell concluded that this perseveration is nonassociative, that is, if uncoupled from the usual rewards systems, animals seek optimal levels of arousal,10,122 and this mediates patterns of alternation and perseveration. Because novel stimuli cause arousal, an animal in a state of high arousal will avoid even mildly novel stimuli even if it would reduce exposure to pain.

“THE COSTS OF PLEASURE AND THE BENEFITS OF PAIN’

Solomon127 proposes an “opponent-process theory of acquired motivation” to explain addictive behavior that originates in frightening or painful events. He points out that frequent exposure to stimuli, pleasant or unpleasant, may lead to habituation; the resulting withdrawal or abstinence state can take on a powerful life of its own and may become an effective source of motivation. In drug addiction, for example, the motivation changes from getting high (pleasure) to controlling a highly aversive withdrawal state.

In contrast with drug-taking, which initially is pleasant, many initially aversive stimuli, such as sauna bathing, marathon running, and parachute jumping, may also be eventually perceived as highly rewarding by people who have repeatedly exposed themselves to these frightening or painful situations. Parachute jumpers, sauna bathers, and marathon runners all feel exhilaration and a sense of well-being from the initially aversive activities. These new sources of pleasure become independent of the fear that was necessary to produce them in the first place. Solomon concludes that certain behaviors can become highly pleasurable: “…if they are derived from aversive processes they can provide a relatively enduring source of positive hedonic tone following the removal of the aversive reinforcer. Fear thus has its positive consequences.”127

Solomon and colleagues have applied these observations to imprinting and social attachment. Their research showed that young animals responded with increasing distress to repeated separations.66 Habituation did not occur, and attachment in fact increased, provided that the imprinting object was presented at fairly regular intervals. Starr130 demonstrated that there is a critical decay duration, the time that it takes for the withdrawal response to the original stimulus to wear off. If the reinforcing stimulus of the imprinting or attachment object is presented at intervals greater than the critical decay duration, increased attachment does not occur. However, animals earlier exposed to repeated separations are more vulnerable to increased distress upon later separations: “repeated exposures to the imprinting object took less time and fewer exposures than did the original exposures.” The strength of the imprinting eventually decays by disuse, but some residues of past experiences remain and facilitate the reactivation of the temporarily dormant system. Readdiction to nicotine and opiates occurs much faster than the initial addiction. If Starr is correct, similar processes account for social attachment to aversive objects and thus “the law of social attachment may be identical to the law of drug addiction.”130

Solomon and coworkers established experimentally that animals and people become habituated to the original stimulus, whether it is morphine, parachute jumping, or marathon running, but the withdrawal syndromes that follow a large number of arousing events retain their integrity over time and recur when the original stimuli are reintroduced.127 Thus, the positive reinforcer loses some of its power, but the negative reinforcer gains power and lasts longer: parachute jumpers continued to feel exhilarated after jumping, even when they feel less year beforehand. Solomon hypothesized that endorphins are secreted in response to certain environmental stresses and play a role in the opponent process. We have recently found evidence that supports this view.

ADDICTION TO TRAUMA

Some traumatized people remain preoccupied with the trauma at the expense of other life experiences137,141 and continue to re-create it in some form for themselves or for others. War veterans may enlist as mercenaries,128 victims of incest may become prostitutes,47,120,125 and victims of childhood physical abuse seemingly provoke subsequent abuse in foster families53 or become self-mutilators143a Still others identify with the aggressor and do to others what was done to them.21,39 Clinically, these people are observed to have a vague sense of apprehension, emptiness, boredom, and anxiety when not involved in activities reminiscent of the trauma. There is no evidence to support Freud’s idea that repetition eventually leads to mastery and resolution. In fact, reliving the trauma repeatedly in psychotherapy may serve to re-enforce the preoccupation and fixation.

Many observers of traumatic bonding have speculated that victims become addicted to their victimizers. Erschak33 asks why the batterer does not stop when injury and pain are apparent and why does the victim not leave? He answers that “they are addicted to each other and to abuse. The system, the interaction, the relation takes hold; the individuals are as powerless as junkies.”

ENDOGENOUS OPIATES AND ATTACHMENT

Thus, Starr,130 Solomon,127 Erschak, and others may be right in postulating that people can become physiologically addicted to each other. There is now considerable evidence that human attachment is, in part, mediated by the endogenous opiate system. Research in non-human primates shows that social attachment is related to the development of core neurobiological functions in the primate brain. Early disruption of the attachment bond causes long-lasting psychobiologic changes that not only reduce the capacity to cope with subsequent social disruption but also disturb parenting processes and create similar vulnerability into the next generation. In recent years knowledge about the brain circuits involved in the maintenance of affiliative behavior are precisely those most richly endowed with opioid receptors.83 Behavioral studies show that the endogenous opioid system plays an important role in the maintenance of social attachment. According to Panksepp and colleagues, the separation response in rats can be inhibited with doses of neuroactive agents to have yielded reliable behavioral effects. Minute injections of morphine abolish both the separation cry in rate infants and the maternal response to it.100,103-105 Morphine-treated mothers (1 mg per kg) disregard male intruders, often attempting no defense of their offspring at all. One mother permitted a male intruder to eat her pups.

Blocking of opioid receptors with naloxone causes increased huddling in nonhuman primates, whereas activation of brain opioid systems can decrease gregariousness.34,104 Lack of caregiving during the first few weeks of life decreases the number of opioid receptors in the cingulate gyrus in mice.13 Panksepp and colleagues have shown that the loss of social support decreases brain opioid activity and produces withdrawal symptoms; emotive circuits mediating loneliness-panic states are apparently activated or disinhibited. Re-establishment of social contact may, among other neural changes, activate endogenous opioid systems, alleviating separation distress and strengthening social bonds.103 If brain opioid activity fulfills social needs, opioid blockade might be expected to influence other forms of gratification, such as sex. Indeed, opioid systems interact with the brain systems that regulate sex-steroid secretion,56 and naloxone facilitates sexual behavior in some mammals.49,96

High levels of stress,3 including social stress,97 also activate opioid systems. Animals exposed to inescapable shock develop stress-induced analgesia (SIA) when re-exposed to stress shortly afterward. This analgesic response is mediated by endogenous opioids and is readily reversible by the opioid receptor blocker naloxone.79 In humans elevations of enkephalins and plasma beta-endorphins have been reported following a large variety of stressors.26,28,73 In testing the generalizability of the phenomenon of SIA to people, we found that seven of eight Vietnam veterans with PTSD showed a 30 percent reduction in perception of pain when viewing a movie depicting combat in Vietnam. This analgesia can be reversed with naloxone.107,143b This amount of analgesia produced by watching 15 minutes of a combat movie was equivalent to that which follows the injection of 8 mg. of morphine. We concluded that Beecher9 was right when, after observing that wounded soldiers require less morphine, he speculated that “strong emotions can block pain” because of the release of endogenous opioids. Our experiments show that even in people traumatized as adults, re-exposure to situations reminiscent of the trauma evokes an endogenous opioid response analogous to that of animals exposed to mild shock subsequent to inescapable shock. Thus, re-exposure to stress may have the same effect as the temporary application of exogenous opioids, providing a similar relief from anxiety.50

Field113 has suggested that normal play and exploratory activity in infants are dependent on the presence of a familiar attachment figure who modulates physiologic arousal by providing a balance between soothing and stimulation. She, Reite,115,116, and others have shown that in the absence of the mother, an infant experiences psychological disorganizing extremes of under- and overarousal. This soothing and arousal may be mediated by alternate stimulation of different neurotransmitter systems, in which the endogenous opioid system is likely to play a role, especially in the subjective experience of safety and soothing. Endogenous opioids decrease central noradrenergic activity,6 and their activation may thus inhibit hyperarousal. Childhood abuse and neglect may cause a long-term vulnerability to be hyperaroused, expressed on a social level as decreased ability to modulate strong affect states. “On a continuum from low to high physiologic arousal, there is an optimal level for every organism. The shape of an individual’s optimal stimulation curve may depend on the level of stimulation received during the early experience.”37 As a result, people who were neglected or abused as children may require much higher external stimulation of the endogenous opioid system for soothing than those whose endogenous opioids can be more easily activated by conditioned responses based on good early caregiving experiences. These victimized people neutralize their hyperarousal by a variety of addictive behaviors including compulsive re-exposure to situations reminiscent of the trauma.

CHILDHOOD TRAUMA, ENDOGENOUS OPIOIDS, AND SELF HARM

If recent animal research is any guide, people, particularly children, who have been exposed to severe, prolonged environmental stress will experience extraordinary increases in both catecholamine and endogenous opioid responses to subsequent stress. The endogenous opioid response may produce both dependence and withdrawal phenomena resembling those of exogenous opioids. This could explain, in part, why childhood trauma is associated with subsequent self-destructive behavior. Depending on which stimuli have come to condition an opioid response, self-destructive behavior may include chronic involvement with abusive partners, sexual masochism, self-starvation, and violence against self or others. In a recent study, we found that patients’ reports of early childhood physical and sexual abuse were highly correlated with self-mutilation and self-starvation in adulthood.143a This controlled study supports numerous other clinical reports about the relationship between childhood abuse and self-destructive behavior.52,106,118 In these people, self-mutilation is a common response to abandonment; it is accompanied by both analgesia and an altered state of consciousness, and it provides relief and returns to normality. The pain, cutting, and burning are apparent attempts at “repairing the cohesiveness of the self in the face of overwhelming anxiety.”35 This pattern is reminiscent of spouse abuse described by Walker:145 “tension gradually builds, an explosive battering (self-mutilating) incident occurs, and a ‘calm, loving respite’ follows.”

Bach-y-Rita7 studied men who were in prison because they habitually took out their frustrations on others violently. He found that they started to self-mutilate in prison when no external object of violence was available. Thus acts of violence that the perpetrator regards as horrible may, in fact, produce somatic calm.

The evidence for the involvement of the endogenous opioid system in self-mutilation is fairly good. A recent study found increased levels of met-enkephalin in habitual self-mutilators during the active stage of self-harm, but not 3 months later.27 Opioid receptor blockade has been found to decrease self-mutilation.60,117 The specific biologic factors that account for the relief felt by these traumatized people who habitually harm themselves or others are still unknown.

TREATMENT IMPLICATIONS

Compulsive repetition of the trauma usually is an unconscious process that, although it may provide a temporary sense of mastery or even pleasure, ultimately perpetuates chronic feelings of helplessness and a subjective sense of being bad and out of control. Gaining control over one’s current life, rather than repeating trauma in action, mood, or somatic states, is the goal of treatment.

Although verbalizing the contextual elements of the trauma is the essence of treatment of acute post-traumatic stress, the essential elements of chronic post-traumatic reactions generally are retrieved with difficulty and often cannot be dealt with until reasonable control over current behavior can assure the safety of both the patient and those in the patient’s immediate surroundings. Failure to approach trauma-related material very gradually leads to an intensification of the effects and physiologic states related to the trauma, leading to increased repetitive phenomena. It is important to keep in mind that the only reason to uncover the trauma is to gain conscious control over the unbidden re-experience or re-enactments. Prior to unearthing the traumatic roots of current behavior, people need to gain reasonable control over the longstanding secondary defenses that were originally elaborated to defend against being overwhelmed by traumatic material such as alcohol and drug abuse and violence against self or others. The trauma can only be worked through after a secure bond is established with another person. The presence of an attachment figure provides people with the security necessary to explore their life experiences and to interrupt the inner or social isolation that keeps people stuck in repetitive patterns. Both the etiology and the cure of trauma-related psychological disturbance depend fundamentally on the security of interpersonal attachments. Once the traumatic experiences have been located in time and place, a person can start making distinctions between current life stresses and past trauma and decrease the impact of the trauma on the present experience.137

Self-help organizations for people with addictions or with backgrounds that include childhood traumas or parental addictions have elaborated a model of treatment that appears to address many of the core issues of repetitive traumatization. These groups provide people with both human attachments and a meaningful cognitive frame for dealing with the sense of helplessness that is central to these problems.. They focus on the development of “serenity,” which can be understood both as a state of automatic stability and of being at peace with one’s surroundings. These groups teach that the way to gain this serenity is by learning to trust, by surrendering, and by making contact and developing interpersonal commitments. They provide a support network that attempts to avoid the barriers that people create to bolster their individual differences, and they thus endeavor to circumvent the shame of being helpless and vulnerable that perpetuates social isolation. Shame and social isolation are thought to promote regression to earlier states of anxious attachment and to addictive involvements. In these circles, it is said that: “No pain is so devastating as the pain a person refuses to face and no suffering is so lasting as suffering left unacknowledged.”23 There is an emphasis on living in the here and now, generally with the acknowledgment that in contrast to victimized children, adults can learn to protect themselves and make a conscious choice about not engaging in relationships or behaviors that are known to be harmful. The underlying assumption is that conclusions drawn from a child’s perspective retain their power into adulthood until verbalized and examined. In a group context, victims can learn that as children they were not responsible for the chaos, violence, and despair surrounding them, but that as adults there are choices and consequences.23,137

These groups also teach that in order to avoid repetition, one has to give up the behavior, drug, or person involved in the addiction. Acknowledging the addictive quality of the involvement is known as overcoming denial. Avoiding acknowledging the feelings promotes acting out. Traumatized people need to understand that acknowledging feelings related to the trauma does not bring back the trauma itself, and its accompanying violence and helplessness. There must be an emphasis on finding replacement activities and experiences that are more rewarding, successful, and powerful in the immediate present. These may include being of help to victims of similar traumas as one’s own.

Psychotropic medicines may be of help to decrease autonomic hyperarousal and decrease all or none of these responses. Lithium, beta-blockers, and serotonin reuptake blockers such as fluoxetine, may be particularly helpful. By decreasing hyperarousal, one decreases the likelihood that current stress will be experienced as a recurrence of past trauma. This facilitates finding solutions appropriate to the current stress rather than the past.139 The use of medications that affect the opioid system should be regarded as experimental and at this time needs to be avoided except in life-threatening cases.

In our last study on patients with borderline personality disorder, Judith Herman and I (unpublished data, 1988) asked our self-mutilating subjects what had helped them most in overcoming the impact of their childhood traumas, including their self-mutilation. All subjects attributed their improvement to having found a safe therapeutic relationship in which they had been able to explore the realities of their childhood experiences and their reactions to them. All subjects reported that they had been able to markedly decrease a variety of repetitive behaviors, including habitual self-harm after they had established a relationship in which they felt safe to acknowledge the realities of both their past and their current lives.

SUMMARY

Trauma can be repeated on behavioral, emotional, physiologic, and neuro endocrinologic levels. Repetition on these different levels causes a large variety of individual and social suffering. Anger directed against the self or others is always a central problem in the lives of people who have been violated and this is itself a repetitive re-enactment of real events from the past.

People need a “safe base” for normal social and biological development. Traumatization occurs when both internal and external resources are inadequate to cope with an external threat. Uncontrollable disruptions or distortions of attachment bonds precede the development of post-traumatic stress syndromes. People seek increased attachment in the face of danger. Adults, as well as children, may develop strong emotional ties with people who intermittently harass, beat, and, threaten them. The persistence of these attachment bonds leads to confusion of pain and love. Assaults lead to hyperarousal states for which the memory can be state-dependent or dissociated, and this memory only returns fully during the renewed terror. This interferes with good judgment about these relationships and allows longing for attachment to overcome realistic fears.

All primates subjected to early abuse and deprivation are vulnerable to engage in violent relationships with peers as adults. Males tend to be hyperaggressive, and females fail to protect themselves and their offspring against danger. Chronic physiologic hyperarousal persists, particularly to stimuli reminiscent of the trauma. Later stresses tend to be experienced as somatic states, rather than as specific events that require specific means of coping. Thus victims of trauma may respond to contemporary stimuli as a return of the trauma, without conscious awareness that past injury rather than current stress is the basis of their physiologic emergency responses. Hyperarousal interferes with the ability to make rational assessments and prevents resolution and integration of the trauma. Disturbances in the catecholamine, serotonin, and endogenous opioid systems have been implicated in this persistence of all-or-nothing responses.

People who have been exposed to highly stressful stimuli develop long-term potentiation of memory tracts that are reactivated at times of subsequent arousal. This activation explains how current stress is experienced as a return of the trauma; it causes a return to earlier behavior patterns. Ordinarily, people will choose the most pleasant of two alternatives. High arousal causes people to engage in familiar behavior, regardless of the rewards. As novel stimuli are anxiety-provoking, under stress, previously traumatized people tend to return to familiar patterns, even if they cause pain.

The “opponent-process theory of acquired motivation” explains how fear may become a pleasurable sensation and that “the laws of social attachment may be identical to those of drug addiction.” Victims can become addicted to their victimizers; social contact may activate endogenous opioid systems, alleviating separation distress and strengthening social bonds. High levels of social stress activate opioid systems as well. Vietnam veterans with PTSD show opioid-mediated reduction in pain perception after re-exposure to a traumatic stimulus. Thus re-exposure to stress can have the same effect as taking exogenous opioids, providing a similar relief from stress.

Childhood abuse and neglect enhance long-term hyperarousal and decreased modulation of strong affect states. Abused children may require much higher external stimulation to affect the endogenous opioid system for soothing than when the biological concomitants of comfort are easily activated by conditioned responses based on good early caregiving experiences. Victimized people may neutralize their hyperarousal by a variety of addictive behaviors, including compulsive re-exposure to victimization of self and others. Gaining control over one’s current life, rather than repeating trauma in action, mood, or somatic states, is the goal of treatment. The only reason to uncover traumatic material is to gain conscious control over unbidden re-experiences or re-enactments. The presence of strong attachments provides people with the security necessary to explore their life experiences and to interrupt the inner or social isolation that keeps them stuck in repetitive patterns. In contrast with victimized children, adults can learn to protect themselves and make conscious choices about not engaging in relationships or behaviors that are harmful.

REFERENCES

1. Ainsworth MDS: Infancy in Uganda: Infant Care and the Growth of Attachment. Baltimore, John Hopkins University Press, 1976
     2. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnosis and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Ed 3. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 1980
     3. Amir S, Brown ZW, Amit Z. The role of endorphins in stress: Evidence and speculations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 4:77-86;1980
     4. Anderson CO, Mason WA: Competitive social strategies in groups of deprived and experienced rhesus monkeys. Dev Psychobiol 11:289-299, 1980
     5. Anisman HL, Ritch M, Sklar LS: Noradrenergic and dopaminergic interactions escape behavior. Psychopharmacology 74:263-268, 1981
     6. Arbila S, Langer SZ: Morphine and beta endorphin inhibit release of noradrenaline from cerebral cortex but not of dopamine from rat striatum. Nature 271:559-560, 1978
     7. Bach-y-Rita: Habitual violence and self-mutilation. Am J Psychiatry 131:1018-1020, 1974 [Abstract]      8. Becker E: The Denial of Death. New York, The Free Press, 1973
     9. Beecher HK: Pain in men wounded in battle. Ann Surg 123:96-105
   10. Berlyne DE: Conflict Arousal in Curiosity. New York, McGraw-Hill, 1960
   11. Bettelheim B: Individual and mass behavior in extreme situations. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 38:417-452, 1943 [Abstract]    12. Blank AS: The unconscious flashback to the war in Vietnam veterans. In Sonnenberg SM, Blank AS, Talbot JA (eds): Stress and Recovery of Vietmam Veterans. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1985
   13. Bonnet KS, Miller JS, Simon EJ: The effects of chronic opiate treatment and social isolation on opiate receptors in the rodent brain. In Kosterlitz HW (ed): Opiate and Endogenous Opioid Peptides. Amsterdam, Elsevier, 1976
   14. Bowlby J: Attachment and Loss. Vol 1: Attachment. New York, Basic Books, 1973
   15. Bowby J: Attachment and Loss. Vol 2: Separation. New York, Basic Books, 1973
   16. Bowby J: Violence in the family as a disorder of the attachment and caregiving systems. Am J Psychoanal 44:9-27, 1984
   17. Brett EA, Ostroff R: Imagery and posttraumatic stress disorder: An overview. Am J Psychiatry 142:417-424, 1985 [Abstract]    18. Brown GL, Ebert ME, Boyer PF, et al: Aggression, suicide and serotonin: Relationships to CSF amine metabolites. Am J Psychiatry 139:741-746, 1982 [Abstract]    19. Bunney WE, Garland BL: Lithium and its possible mode of action. In Post RM, Ballenger JC (eds): Neurobiology of Mood Disorders. Baltimore, Williams and Wilkins, 1984
   20. Burgess AW, Hartman CR, McCormack A: Abused to abuser: Antecedents of socially deviant behavior. Am J Psychiatry 144:1431-1436, 1987 [Abstract]    21. Burgstein A: Posttraumatic flashbacks, dream disturbances and mental imagery. J Clin Psychiatry 46:374-378, 1985
   22. Carmen EH, Reiker PP, Mills T: Victims of violence and psychiatric illness. Am J Psychiatry 141:378-379, 1984 [Abstract]    23. Cermak TL, Brown S: Interactional group therapy with the adult children of alcoholics. Int J Group Psychother 32:375-389, 1982
   24. Cicchetti D: The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Dev 55:1-7, 1984
   25. Coe CL, Wiener S, Rosenberg LT, et al: Endocrine and immune response tos to separation and maternal loss in nonhuman primates. In Reite M, Fields T (eds): The Psychobiology of Attachment and Separation. Orlando, Academic Press, 1985
   26. Cohen MR, Pinchas M, et al: Stress induced plasma endorphin immunoreactivity may predict postoperative morphine usage. Psychiatry Res 6:7-12, 1982 [Medline]    27. Cold J, Allolio B, Rees LH: Raised plasma metenkephalin in patients who habitually mutilate themselves. Lancet 2:545-546, 1983 [Medline]    28. Colt EW, Wardlaw SL, Frantz AG: The effect of running on plasma beta endorphin. Life Sci 28:1637-1640, 1981
   29. Cooper AM: Masochism: In Glick RA, Meyers DI (eds): Current Psychological Perspectives. Hillsdale, The Analytic Press, 1988
   30. Delaney R, Tussi D, Gold PE: Longterm potentiation as a neurophysiological analog of memory. Pharmocol Biochem Behav 18:137-139, 1983
   31. Dutton D, Painter SL: Traumatic bonding: The development of emotional attachments in battered women and other relationships of intermittent abuse. Victimology 6:139-155, 1981
   32. Eich JE: The cue-dependent nature of state dependent retrival. Memory Cognition 8:157-168, 1980
   33. Erschak GM: The escalation and maintenance of spouse abuse: A cybernetic model. Victimology 9:247-253, 1984
   34. Fabre-Nys C, Meller RE, Keverne EG: Opiate antogonists stimulate affiliative behavior in monkeys. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 18:137-139, 1983
   35. Ferenczi S: Confusion of tongues between the adult and the child: The language of tenderness and the language of passion. In Ferenczi S: Problems and Methods of Psychoanalysis. London, Hogarth Press, 1955
   36. Field T: Attachment of psychobiological attunement: Being on the same wavelength. In Reite M, Fields T (eds): The Psychobiology of Attachment and Separation. Orlando, Academic Press, 1985
   37. Field T: Interaction and attachment in normal and atypical infants. J Consult Clin Psychol 55:1-7, 1987 [Abstract]    38. Finkelhor D, Brown A: The traumatic nature of child sexual abuse. Am J Orthopsychiatry 55:530-541, 1985
   39. Fish Murray CC, Koby EV, van der Kolk BA: Evolving ideas: The effect of abuse on children’s thought. In van der Kolk BA (ed): Psychological Trauma. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1987
   40. Fleming JB: Stopping Wife Abuse. Garden City, Anchor Books, 1979
   41. Fox RP, Narcissistic rage and the problem of combat aggression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 311:807-811, 1974
   42. Freud S: Moses and Monotheism (1939). In Complete Psychological Works. Vol 18. Translated and edited by J Strachey. London, Hogarth Presss, 1954
   43. Freud S: The aetiology of hysteria (1896). In Complete Psychological Works, Standard Ed. Vol 3 Translated and edited by J Strachey. London, Hogarth Press, 1954
   44. Freud S: Beyond the pleasure principle (1920). In Complete Psychological Works, Standard Ed. Vol 3 Translated and edited by J Strachey. London, Hogarth Press, 1954
   45. Freud S: Group psychology and analysis of the ego (1921). In Complete Psychological Works, Standard Ed. Vol 18. Translated and edited by J Strachey. London, Hogarth Press, 1955
   46. Freize I: Investigating the causes and consequences of marital rape. J Women Culture Soc 8:532-553, 1983
   47. Gelinas DJ: The persistent negative effects of incest. Psychiatry 46:312-332, 1983
   48. Gelles RJ: The Violent Home. Beverly Hills, Sage Publications, 1972
   49. Gessa G, Paglietta E, Pellegrini-Quarantotty B: Induction of copulatory behavor in sexually inactive rats by naxolone. Science 204:203-205, 1979
   50. Gold M, Pottash AC, Sweeney D, et al: Antimanic, anti-depressant and antipanic effects of opiates: Clinical neuroanatomical and biochemical evidence. Ann NY Acad Sci 398:140-150, 1982
   51. Gold PE, Zornetzer SF: The mnemom and its juices: Neuromodulation of memory processes. Behav Neural Biol 38:151-189, 1983
   52. Graf H, Mallin R: The syndrome of the wrist cutter. Am J Psychiatry 124:36-42, 1967 [Abstract]    53. Green AH: Child Maltreatment. New York, Jason Aronson, 1980
   54. Green AH: Self-destructive behavior in battered children. Am J Psychiatry 135:579-582, 1978 [Medline]    55. Groth AN: Sexual trauma in the life histories of sex offenders. Victomology 4:6-10, 1979
   56. Hahn EF, Fishman J: Changes in rat brain opiate receptor content upon castration and testosterone replacement. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 90:819-823
   57. Haley SA: When the patient reports atrocities: Specific treatment considerations of the Vietnam veteran. Arch Gen Psychiatry 30:191-196, 1974
   58. Harlow HF, Harlow MK: Psychopathology in monkeys. In Kimmel HD (ed): Experimental Psychopathology. New York, Academic Press, 1971
   59. Hendin H, Pollinger-Haas A, Singer P: The influence of pre-combat personality on posttraumatic stress disorders. Compr Psychiatry 24:530-534, 1983
   60. Herman BH, Hammock MK, Arthur-Smith A, et al. Naltrexone decreases self injurious behavior. Ann Neurol 22:550-552, 1987
   61. Herman JL: Father Daughter Incest. Cambridge, Harvard University Press, 1981
   62. Hilberman E: Overview: The wife-beater’s wife reconsidered. Am J Psychiatry 137:1336-1347; 1980 [Abstract]    63. Hilberman E, Munson M: Sixty battered women. Victimology 2:460-471, 1978
   64. Hilgard JR: Anniversary reactions in parents precipitated by children. Psychiatry 16:73-80, 1953
   65. Hilgard JR: Depressive and psychotic states as anniversaries to sibling death in childhood. Int Psychiatry Clin 6:197-211, 1969
   66. Hoffman RS, Ratner AM: A reinforcement model of imprinting: Implications for socialization in monkeys and men. Psychol Rev 80: 527-524, 1973
   67. Holen A The long-term psychological effects of an oilrig disaster. Paper Presented at the Fourth Annual Conference of the Society for Traumatic Stress Studies. Baltimore, 1987
   68. Horowitz M, Wilner N, Kaltrider N: Signs and symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 37:85-92, 1980 [Abstract]    69. Horowitz MJ: Stress Response Syndromes. Ed 2, New York, Jason Aronson, 1986
   70. Horowitz MJ, Becker SS: The compulsion to repeat trauma: Experimental study of intrusive thinking after stress. J Nerv Ment Dis 153:32-40, 1971
   71. Jaffe P, Wolfe D, Wilson SK, et al: Family violence and child adjustment: A comparative analysis of girls’ and boys’ behavioral symptoms. Am J Psychiatry 143:74-77, 1986 [Abstract]    72. James J, Meyerding J: Early sexual experiences as a factor in prostitution. Arch Sex Behav 7:31-42, 1977
   73. Janal MN, Colt EWD, Clark WC, et al. Pain sensitivity, mood and plasma endocrine levels in man following long-distance running: Effects of naxolone. Pain 19:13-25, 1984 [Medline]    74. Janet P: The Major Symptoms of Hysteria. London and New York, Macmillan, 1907
   75. Janet P: L’Automatisme Psychologique. Paris, Alcan, 1889
   76. Janet P: The Mental State of Hystericals. Paris, Alcan, 1911
   77. Kagan J, Reznick S, Snidman N: The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Dev 58: 1459-1473, 1987 [Medline]    78. Kardiner A: The Traumatic Neuroses of War. New York, P. Hoeber, 1941
   79. Kelly DD: The role of endorphins in stress-related analgesia. Ann NY Acad Sci 398::260-271
   80. Kempe RS, Kempe CH: Child Abuse. Cambridge, Harvard University Press, 1978
   81. Kihlstrom JF: Conscious, subconscious, unconscious: A cognitive perspective. In Bowers KS, Meichenbaum D (eds): The Unconscious Reconsidered. New York, John Wiley and Sons, 1984
   82. Kilpatrick DG, Veronen LJ, Best CL: Factors predicting psychological distress in rape victims. In Figley C (ed): Trauma and Its Wake. New York, Brunner/Mazel, 1985
   83. Kling A,Steklis HD: A neural substrate for affliative behavior in non-human primates. Brain Behav Evol 13:216-238, 1976
   84. Kolb L: Neuropsychological hypothesis explaining posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 144:989-995. 1987 [Abstract]    85. Kraemer GW: Causes of changes in brain noradrenaline systems and later effects on responses to social stressors in rhesus monkeys: The cascade hypothesis. In Antidepressants and Receptor Function, Wiley, Chichester (Ciba Foundation Symposium 123), 1986
   86. Kraemer GW: Effects of differences in early social experiences on primate neurobiological behavioral development. In Reite M, Fields T (eds): The Psychobiology of Attachment and Separation. Orlando, Academic Press, 1985
   87. Krugman S: Trauma and the family: Perspectives on the intergenerational transmission of violence. In van der Kolk BA: Psychological Trauma. Washington DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1987
   88. Krystal H: Trauma and affects. Psychoanal Study Child 33:81-116, 1978
   89. Lewis D, Balla D: Delinguency and psychopathology. New York, Grune and Stratton, 1976
   90. Lewis D, Pincus J, Bard B et al: Neuropsychiatric, psychoeducational and family characteristics of 14 juveniles condemned to death in the United States, Am J Psychiatry 145:584-589, 1988 [Abstract]    91. Lewis D, Shanok SS, Pincus JH, et al: Violent juvenile delinquents: Psychiatric, neurological, psychological and abuse factors. J Child Psychiatry 18:307-319, 1979
   92. Lindy J: Vietnam: A Casebook. New York, Brunner/Mazel, 1987
   93. Maclean PD: Brain evolution relating to family, play and the separation call. Arch Gen Psychiatry 42;505-517, 1985 [Abstract]    94. Maier SF, Seligman MEP: Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence. J Exp Psychol Center 105:3-46, 1976
   95. Mason WA Early social deprivation in the non-human primates: Implications for human behavior. In Glass FT (ed): Environmental Influences. New York, Rockefeller University Press, 1968
   96. McIntosh TK, Vallano ML, Barfield RJ: Effects of morphine beta-endorphin and naloxone on catecholamine levels and sexual behavior in the male rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 13:435-441, 1980
   97. Miczek KA, Thompson ML, Shuster L: Opioid-like analgesia in defeated mice. Science 215:1520-1522, 1982
   98. Mitchell D, Koleszar aS, Scopatz RA: Arousal and T-Maze choice behavior in mice: a convergent paradigm for neophobia constructs and optimal arousal theory. Learn Motiv 15:287-301, 1984
   99. Mitchell D, Osborne EW, O’Boyle MW: Habituation under stress: Shocked mice show nonassociative learning in a T-maze. Behav Neural Biol 43:212-217, 1985
100. Newman JD, Murphy MR, Harbough CR: Naxolone-reversible suppression of isolation call production after morphine injections in squirrel monkeys. Soc Neurosci Abstr 8:940, 1982
101. Novak MA, Harlow HF: Social Recovery of Monkeys isolated for the first year of life: Long-term assessment. Dev Psychol 15:50-61, 1979
102. Ochberg FM, Soskis DA: Victims of Terrorism. Boulder, Westview, 1982
103. Panksepp J: Toward a more general psychobiological theory of emotions. Behav Brain Sc 5:407-468, 1982
104. Panksepp J, Najam N, Soares F: Morphine reduces social cohesion in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 11:131-134, 1979
105. Panksepp J, Sivey SM, Normansell LA: Brain opioids and social emotions. In Reite M, Fields T (eds): The Psychobiology of Attachment and Separation. Orlando, Academic Press, 198
106. Pattison EM, Kahan J: The deliberate self-harm syndrome. Am J Psychiatry 140:867-872, 1983 [Medline] 107. Pitman R, Orr S, Laforque D, et al: Psychophysiology of PTSD imagery in Vietnam combat veterans. Arch Gen Psychiatry 44:940-976, 1987
108. Pittman R, Orr S, van der Kolk BA, et al: Opioid mediated stress induced analgesia in Vietnam combat veterans with PTSD. Unpublished manuscript, 1989
109. Pollock GH: Anniversary reactions: Trauma and mourning. Psychoanaly Q 39:347-371, 1970
110. Rainey JM, Aleem A, Ortiz A, et al: Laboratory procedures for the inducement of flashbacks. Am J Psychiatry 144:1317-1319, 1987 [Abstract] 111. Rajecki DW, Lamb ME, Obmascher P: Toward a general theory of infantile attachment: A comparative review of aspects of the social bond. Behav Brain Sci 3:417-464, 1978
112. Rangell L: Discussion of the Buffalo Creek disaster: the course of psychic trauma. Am J Psychiatry 133:313-316, 1976 [Abstract] 113. Reiker PP, Carmen E(H): The vicim to patient process: The disconfirmation and transformation of abuse. Am J Orthopsychiatry 56:360-370. 1986
114. Reite M, Field T: The Psychobiology of Attachment and Separation, Orlando, Academic Press, 1985
115. Reite M, Short R, Seiler C: Attachment, Loss and Depression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 22:141-169, 1981
116. Reite M, Short R, Seiler C: Physiological correlates of separation in surrogate reared infants: A study in altered attachment bonds. Dev Psychobiol 11:427-435, 1978
117. Richardson JS, Zaleski WA: Naxolone and self-mutilation. Biol Psychiatry 18:99-101, 1983 [Medline] 118. Rosenthal RJ, Rinzler C, Wallsh R, et al: Wrist-cutting syndrome: The meaning of a gesture. Am J Psychiatry 128:47-52, 1972
119. Rounsaville B, lifton N, Bieber M: The natural history of a psychotherapy group for battered wives. Psychiatry 42;63-78, 1978
120. Russell D: The Secret Trauma. New York, Basic Books, 1986
121. Seghorn TK, Boucher RJ, Prentky RA: Childhood sexual abuse in the lives of sexually aggressive offenders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 26:262-267, 1987
122. Sheldon AB: Preference for familiar vs. novel stimuli as a function of the familiarity of the environment. J Comp Physiol Psychol 67:516-521, 169
123. Sherman AD, Petty F: Neurochemical basis of the action of antidepressants on learned helplessness. Behav Neural Biol 30:119-134, 1980
124. Shore JH, Tatum EL, Vollmer WM: Psychiatric reactions to disaster: The Mount St. Helens experience. Am J Psychiatry 143:590-595, 1986 [Abstract] 125. Silbert MD, Pines AM: Sexual child abuse as an antecedent to prostitution. Child Abuse Negl 5:407-411, 1981
126. Simpson CA, Porter GL: Self-mutilation in children and adolescents. Bull Menninger Clin 45:428-438, 1981
127. Solomon RL: The opponent-process theory of acquired motivation: The costs of pleasure and the benefits of pain. Am Psychol 35:691-712, 1980
128. Solursh L: Combat addiction: Implications in symptom maintenance and treatment planning. Paper Presented at the Third Annual Meeting of the Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, Baltimore, Maryland, 1987
129. Squire LR: Memory and the Brain. New York, Oxford University Press, 1987
130. Starr MD: An opponent process of motivation. VI: Time and intensity variables in the development of separation-induced distress calling in ducklings. J Exp Psychol (Animal Behav) 4:338-355; 1978
131. Stoddard F: Stress disorders in burned out children and adolescents. Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Psychiatric Association. Dallas, 1985
132. Strauss MA: Sociological perspective on the prevention of wife-beating. In Roy M (ed): Battered Women: A Psychosociological Study of Domestic Violence, New York, Van Nostrand Reinhold, 1977
133. Strian F, Klipcera C: Die Bedeuting psychoautonomische Reaktionen im Entstehung und Persisten von Angstzusttanden. Nervenartzt 49:576-583, 1978
134. Suomi SJ: The development of affect in Rhesus monkeys. In Fox N, Davidson R (eds): The Psychology of Affective Development. Hillsdale, New Jersey, Lawrence Erlbaum, 1984
135. Suomi SJ, Eisele CD, Grady S, et al: Depressive behavior in adult monkeys following separation from family environment. J Abnorm Psychol 84:576-578, 1978
136. Terr L: What happens to early memories of trauma? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1:96-104, 1988
137. van der Kolk B: Psychological Trauma. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1987
138. van der Kolk BA: Adolescent vulnerability to post traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry 48:365-370, 1985
139. van der Kolk BA, The drug treatment of PTSD. J Affect Disord 13:203-213, 1987
140. van der Kolk BA, Post traumatic stress disorder in men: The impact on the family. In Strauss M (ed): Abuse and Victimization: A Life Span Perspective. Baltimore, Johns Hopkins University Press, 1988
141. van der Kolk, BA: The trauma spectrum: the interaction of biological and social events in the genesis of the trauma response. J Traum Stress 1:273-290, 1988 [Abstract] 142. van der Kolk BA, Ducey CP: Clinical implications of the Rorschach in posttraumatic stress. In van der Kolk BA (ed) Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Psychological and Biological Sequelae. Washington, DC, APA Press, 1984
143. van der Kolk BA, Greenberg MS, Boyd H, et al: Inescapable shock, neurotransmitters and addiction to trauma. Towards a psychobiology of post traumatic stress, Biol Psychiatry 20:414-325, 1985
143a. van der Kolk B, Herman J, Perry J: Childhood trauma and self destructive behavior in adulthood. Unpublished data, 1988
143b van der Kolk BA, Greenburg MS, Orr S, et al. Pain perception and engogenous opioids in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychopharmacol Bull 25:1989
144. Visitaner MA. Volpicelli JR, Seligman MEP: Tumor rejection in rats after inescapable shock. Science 216:437-439, 1982
145. Walker L: The Battered Woman. New York, Harper and Row, 1979
146. Weiss JM, Glazer HI, Pohorecky LA, et al: Effects of chronic exposure to stressors on subsequent avoidance-escape behavior and on brain norpinephrine. Psychosom Med 37:522-524, 1975
147. Winnicott DW: Maturational Processes and the Facilitating Environment: Studies in the Theory of Emotional Development. New York, International Universities Press, 1965

Massachusetts Mental Health Center
Harvard Medical School
74 Fenwood Road
Boston Massachusetts 02116


*Director, Trauma Center, Massachusetts Mental Health Center, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts

TAGS: SCARS, Information About Scams, Anti-Scam, Scams, Scammers, Fraudsters, Cybercrime, Crybercriminals, Romance Scams, Scam Victims, Online Fraud, Online Crime Is Real Crime, Psychology of Scams, Mental Health, Understanding Trauma Compulsions, Compulsion of Repeating Trauma, Victim Trauma, PTSD,

PLEASE SHARE OUR ARTICLES WITH YOUR FRIENDS & FAMILY

HELP OTHERS STAY SAFE ONLINE – YOUR KNOWLEDGE CAN MAKE THE DIFFERENCE!
THE NEXT VICTIM MIGHT BE YOUR OWN FAMILY MEMBER OR BEST FRIEND!

SCARS the Society of Citizens Against Relationship Scams Incorporated

By the SCARS™ Editorial Team
Society of Citizens Against Relationship Scams Inc.

A Worldwide Crime Victims Assistance & Crime Prevention Nonprofit Organization Headquartered In Miami Florida USA & Monterrey NL Mexico, with Partners In More Than 60 Countries
To Learn More, Volunteer, or Donate Visit: www.AgainstScams.org
Contact Us: Contact@AgainstScams.org